{"created":"2024-10-31T01:30:47.034829+00:00","id":2019964,"links":{},"metadata":{"_buckets":{"deposit":"80a522b5-f53c-4cba-a4f2-2f669bd2d30d"},"_deposit":{"created_by":7,"id":"2019964","owners":[7],"pid":{"revision_id":0,"type":"depid","value":"2019964"},"status":"published"},"_oai":{"id":"oai:ocu-omu.repo.nii.ac.jp:02019964","sets":["1623632832836:1721355153493"]},"author_link":[],"item_1617186331708":{"attribute_name":"タイトル","attribute_value_mlt":[{"subitem_title":"Explanations for cultural differences in thinking: Easterners' dialectical thinking and Westerners' linear thinking","subitem_title_language":"en"}]},"item_1617186419668":{"attribute_name":"作成者","attribute_type":"creator","attribute_value_mlt":[{"creatorAffiliations":[{"affiliationNames":[{"affiliationName":"Osaka City University","affiliationNameLang":"en"}]}],"creatorNames":[{"creatorName":"Yama, Hiroshi","creatorNameLang":"en"},{"creatorName":"ヤマ, ヒロシ","creatorNameLang":"ja-Kana"},{"creatorName":"山, 祐嗣","creatorNameLang":"ja"}],"nameIdentifiers":[{"nameIdentifier":"1000080202373","nameIdentifierScheme":"KAKEN2","nameIdentifierURI":"https://nrid.nii.ac.jp/nrid/1000080202373"},{"nameIdentifier":"0000-0003-1612-3550","nameIdentifierScheme":"ORCID","nameIdentifierURI":"https://orcid.org/0000-0003-1612-3550"}]},{"creatorAffiliations":[{"affiliationNames":[{"affiliationName":"University of Wollongong in Dubai","affiliationNameLang":"en"}]}],"creatorNames":[{"creatorName":"Zakaria, Norhayati","creatorNameLang":"en"}],"nameIdentifiers":[{"nameIdentifier":"0000-0002-3970-1694","nameIdentifierScheme":"ORCID","nameIdentifierURI":"https://orcid.org/0000-0002-3970-1694"}]}]},"item_1617186499011":{"attribute_name":"権利情報","attribute_value_mlt":[{"subitem_rights":"This is an Accepted Manuscript of an article published by Taylor & Francis in Journal of Cognitive Psychology on 04/06/2019, available online: https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2019.1626862","subitem_rights_language":"en"}]},"item_1617186609386":{"attribute_name":"主題","attribute_value_mlt":[{"subitem_subject":"弁証法的思考","subitem_subject_language":"ja","subitem_subject_scheme":"Other"},{"subitem_subject":"高コンテクスト文化と低コンテクスト文化","subitem_subject_language":"ja","subitem_subject_scheme":"Other"},{"subitem_subject":"cultural difference","subitem_subject_language":"en","subitem_subject_scheme":"Other"},{"subitem_subject":"dialectical thinking","subitem_subject_language":"en","subitem_subject_scheme":"Other"},{"subitem_subject":"low context culture and high context culture","subitem_subject_language":"en","subitem_subject_scheme":"Other"},{"subitem_subject":"socioecological approach","subitem_subject_language":"en","subitem_subject_scheme":"Other"}]},"item_1617186626617":{"attribute_name":"内容記述","attribute_value_mlt":[{"subitem_description":"東洋人は、西洋人と比較して、矛盾があってもそれを 受容しやすいという弁証法的傾向が強いとされていますが、これまでは、この違いは、東洋の集団主義文化・西洋の個人主義文化という枠組みで説明されていました。つまり、東洋人は集団主義文化の中で調和を重んじ、対立あるいは矛盾する主張があっても集団の調和を崩さないように弁証法的になるという説明です。しかし、上記の通説では説明のつかない事例が存在することから、東洋の高コンテクスト文化・西洋の低コンテクスト文化という 枠組みで説明を試みました。日本は「阿吽の呼吸」がよしとされる典型的な高コンテクスト文化です。つまり 日本を含めた東洋では、コミュニケーションが コンテクストに依存する度合いが高いため、少々の矛盾が含まれていても暗黙裡にそれを解消できるとする規範が形成されているとする説明です。この説明は、地勢的・生態的要因から文化多様性をとらえるビッグ・ヒストリーとも結びつけられ、また、日本と中国の差異の説明にも適用されています。","subitem_description_language":"ja","subitem_description_type":"Abstract"},{"subitem_description":"Since Easterners' naïve dialectical thinking, which is contrasted with Westerners' linear thinking, introduced, many cross-cultural studies on human thinking have been conducted and explanations for the cultural differences have been proposed. First, after examining the robustness of these cultural differences, two existing explanations are discussed in this paper. The first is based on the distinction between Westerners' analytic cognition and Easterners' holistic cognition....","subitem_description_language":"en","subitem_description_type":"Abstract"},{"subitem_description":"This research has been supported by a CHORUS grant from the French Ministry of Research and the Japanese Society for the Promotion of Science and by a grant-in-aid from the Japanese Society for the Promotion of Science (No. 23330199 and No. 18K03010).","subitem_description_language":"en","subitem_description_type":"Other"}]},"item_1617186643794":{"attribute_name":"出版者","attribute_value_mlt":[{"subitem_publisher":"Taylor & Francis","subitem_publisher_language":"en"}]},"item_1617186783814":{"attribute_name":"識別子","attribute_value_mlt":[{"subitem_identifier_type":"DOI","subitem_identifier_uri":"https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2019.1626862"}]},"item_1617186920753":{"attribute_name":"収録物識別子","attribute_value_mlt":[{"subitem_source_identifier":"2044-592X","subitem_source_identifier_type":"ISSN"}]},"item_1617187056579":{"attribute_name":"書誌情報","attribute_value_mlt":[{"bibliographicIssueDates":{"bibliographicIssueDate":"2019-06-04","bibliographicIssueDateType":"Issued"},"bibliographicIssueNumber":"4","bibliographicPageEnd":"506","bibliographicPageStart":"487","bibliographicVolumeNumber":"31","bibliographic_titles":[{"bibliographic_title":"Journal of Cognitive Psychology","bibliographic_titleLang":"en"}]}]},"item_1617258105262":{"attribute_name":"資源タイプ","attribute_value_mlt":[{"resourcetype":"journal article","resourceuri":"http://purl.org/coar/resource_type/c_6501"}]},"item_1617265215918":{"attribute_name":"出版タイプ","attribute_value_mlt":[{"subitem_version_resource":"http://purl.org/coar/version/c_b1a7d7d4d402bcce","subitem_version_type":"AO"}]},"item_1617353299429":{"attribute_name":"関連情報","attribute_value_mlt":[{"subitem_relation_type":"isVersionOf","subitem_relation_type_id":{"subitem_relation_type_id_text":"https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2019.1626862","subitem_relation_type_select":"DOI"}}]},"item_1722232185104":{"attribute_name":"ファイル情報","attribute_type":"file","attribute_value_mlt":[{"accessrole":"open_date","date":[{"dateType":"Available","dateValue":"2020-07-02"}],"displaytype":"detail","filename":"2044592x-31-4-487.pdf","filesize":[{"value":"1.1 MB"}],"format":"application/pdf","mimetype":"application/pdf","url":{"label":"本文","objectType":"fulltext","url":"https://ocu-omu.repo.nii.ac.jp/record/2019964/files/2044592x-31-4-487.pdf"},"version_id":"643c0a88-0c1f-46f6-bb4c-357a6823bbaa"}]},"item_1722577087681":{"attribute_name":"記事種別等","attribute_value_mlt":[{"subitem_description":"Article","subitem_description_language":"en","subitem_description_type":"Other"}]},"item_1725590125188":{"attribute_name":"言語","attribute_value_mlt":[{"subitem_language":"eng"}]},"item_title":"Explanations for cultural differences in thinking: Easterners' dialectical thinking and Westerners' linear thinking","item_type_id":"40002","owner":"7","path":["1721355153493"],"pubdate":{"attribute_name":"PubDate","attribute_value":"2020-07-02"},"publish_date":"2020-07-02","publish_status":"0","recid":"2019964","relation_version_is_last":true,"title":["Explanations for cultural differences in thinking: Easterners' dialectical thinking and Westerners' linear thinking"],"weko_creator_id":"7","weko_shared_id":-1},"updated":"2024-10-31T01:30:58.757789+00:00"}